Exploring L2 Learning Potential through Computerized Dynamic Assessment

Document Type : Original Article

Authors

1 Department of English, Razi University, Kermanshah, Iran

2 Department of English Language and Literature, Razi University, Kermanshah, Iran

Abstract

 
Dynamic Assessment (DA) is theoretically derived from Vygotsky's Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD). This study seeks to investigate the effect of Computerized Dynamic Assessment (C-DA) on assisting learners to realize their latent potential in learning reading comprehension. A group of 32 adult EFL students studying English as their BA major in a University in Iran participated in the study. In order to measure the students' potential for learning, Kozulin and Garb's (2002) formula which is called the Learning Potential Score (LPS) was used. The LPSs represented how much mediation was likely required for an individual or a group of individuals to develop or move forward. The results showed that learners with the same pretest scores might turn out to have different or even drastically different DA posttest scores and hence different LPSs. On account of the LPS obtained through the C-DA principles at-risk students are provided with opportunities that might result in overcoming some of the challenges they faced in traditional testing.

Keywords


1. Introduction

Derived from the writings of prominent Russian psychologist, Vygotsky (1896-1934), Dynamic Assessment (DA) focuses on learners' potential for future learning and development (Lidz, 1987 as cited in Ebadi, 2014) and seeks to enable individual learners in its inchoate period and more cohorts of individuals over time to outperform their current performance level by making use of mediation which is sensitized to the needs of learners. The theoretical basis of DA, namely, Sociocultural Theory of Mind (SCT) was developed by Vygotsky (1987) which proposes that learners will be cognizant of their potential abilities owing to taking part in some organized activities in which mediation plays a key role.

In recent years, the growing importance of DA in L2 (Ableeva, 2008; Aljaafreh & Lantolf, 1994; Poehner & Lantolf, 2005; Poehner, 2008) in general, and L2 reading comprehension (Antón, 2009; Kozulin & Garb, 2002, 2004) in particular has been recognized. Improving students' progress in certain abilities, for example, reading comprehension in this study, and to make them aware of their potentiality for learning by focusing on the cognitive processes of learning are two principal purposes of DA. As it is also been confirmed by (Dörfler, Golke, & Artelt, 2009) for the development of tasks and feedbacks within dynamic reading-competence tests, a conception of inferential processes and interventions is required to construct and apply suitable instructional techniques. Hence, "dynamic assessments in the domain of reading comprehension rely on instruction and practice in metacognitive knowledge (including strategies) which is specific to certain reading tasks and goals" (Dörfler, et al., p. 80). That is why the 'reading comprehension' skill has been chosen to be examined in this study and its first question is dedicated to exploring the effects of C-DA on promoting Iranian EFL students' reading strategies.

A new type of DA is Computerized Dynamic Assessment (C-DA) which attempts to overcome some of its limitations such as being time-consuming and focusing on low number of participants at a time through presenting a workable solution to the above-mentioned problems by a computer software program, in this study CDRT developed by Pishghadam and Barabadi (2012). Yet, these are not the only shortcomings of DA which have been overcome utilizing the C-DA procedures. Haywood and Tzuriel (2002), Haywood and Lidz (2007) and Poehner (2008) all agree upon two more shortcomings of DA: first, in addition to being time-consuming, it needs a hyperactive and energetic teacher (mediator) to take charge of such classes. Second, DA practitioners worry about its reliability and validity. Of course, Pishghadam and Barabadi (2012) have done a study which alleviated the concerns in terms of DA reliability and validity. In addition, because most of the English classes in Iran are large in size, applying the DA procedure (i.e., providing human-to-human mediation to each individual learner
can be unrealistic). Unknown number of instruction sessions or not having "equal time-on-task in DA experimental designs” is another problem of DA because in research viewpoint it is preferred to have a certain number showing equal time-on-task" (Nassaji, & Swain, 2000, p. 48).

DA is different from nondynamic assessment (NDA) in some regards. One of the differences between dynamic and nondynamic assessment is in their orientation, that is, they are process-oriented and product-oriented respectively (Carney & Cioffi, 1992). While citing the aforementioned study based on which DA differs from other traditional assessment with regard to orientation, procedure and interpretation, Birjandi, Estaji, and Deyhim (2013, p. 61) stated that "DA focuses on the evaluation process as well as the product." The researchers, therefore, chose DA as the assessment type of his study due to its feasibility in dealing with process and product.

Another reason for preferring DA over NDA in this study is the opportunity which DA provides for students to receive mediation that assists them to gain their potential learning. In contrary to NDA which considers individuals born with a certain intelligence that remains fixed throughout life (Feuerstein & Falik, 2010), DA takes individuals' modifiability and potential to change into consideration and believes that they will improve to their best of their potential if they are provided with an appropriately-mediated learning environment (Teo, 2012).

There are generally two approaches to do DA (i.e., interventionist & interactionist). In the interventionist approach to DA, the same mediation is used with every learner. Therefore, it is easier to manage a larger number of participants (Poehner, 2008). In the interactionist approach to DA, the mediator cooperates separately with each learner to coconstruct ZPDs during different one-on-one sessions and the mediation provided for each student may be (is) different from the one provided for the others (Aljaafreh & Lantolf, 1994). It is true that a larger number of students can be examined in Interventionist DA in comparison to the number to be investigated in Interactionist DA. Because the number of participants in the this study is more than those in other studies, such as Aljaafreh and Lantolf (1994), Antón (2009), to name just a few, the interventionist approach to DA was preferred over the interactionist one.

To overcome some of the problems of DA such as the problem of the participants' age and number mentioned earlier, the researchers found out that carrying out this study on the area of dynamic assessment utilizing computer which is technically called C-DA can be more helpful in repaying some of the criticisms faced with DA. The problem of the number of participants and the number of skills that are dynamically assessed in a single DA procedure emanates from the sensitivity to each individual learner’s ZPD (Pishghadam, Barabadi, & Kamrood, 2011). To deal with these problems, DA researchers have recently sought help from technology (computer software) to take charge of the mediators' responsibilities, in order to be able to assess a greater number of participants and skills in a single DA procedure (Jacobs, 2001; Tzuriel & Shamir, 2002; Summers, 2008; Pishghadam et al., 2011). This study utilizes Computerized Dynamic Reading Comprehension Test (CDRT) because this software can show those questions which have not been answered by students, thus it helps the interventionist mediator explore the students' problems and solve them by applying appropriate strategies.

This study attempts to explore the effects of C-DA on promoting at-risk adult Iranian EFL students' learning potential in terms of different reading strategies. More cognitively-demanding reading skills (Teo, 2012), such as finding main idea in higher-level texts, using contextual clues to predict the meaning of words, and making inferences or more technically strategy instruction were also included in this study. The importance of strategy training has been highlighted by some researchers. For example, Dole, Brown, and Trathen (1996) carried out a study on the impacts of strategy instruction on the comprehension performance among a group of at-risk students indicating the outperformance of the group which received strategy training when they were asked to read some selections without any external assistance, or on their own. This study showed that those who were lower achievers benefited particularly from learning specific strategies. Additionally, they showed that strategies could compensate for lack of background knowledge. DA whose procedures meet the needs of reading assessment is concomitant with underscoring the shift from rote learning to strategy instruction (Kozulin & Garb, 2002). With regard to strategy teaching phase which is called Enrichment Program (EP) in dynamic assessment, Kozulin and Garb, stated that it "included preteaching difficult vocabulary, activating prior knowledge, providing directions for reading, revealing system and structure in text and requiring articulation of what is learned" (p. 116). Because mediators (teachers) have to interact with students during the mediation in EP, they will be able to find their students' areas of difficulty. Hence, they can help the learners overcome their problems by providing sufficient feedback and even mediators can motivate learners to do their best to achieve their best latent potential by providing a picture of their abilities. This can even be more tangible while dealing with reading and reading strategies because if the mediator helps learners apply the reading strategies appropriately, they can to a large extent maintain the same strategies even after the mediation.

This study sought to investigate the effect of (C-DA) - which is a type of DA- on assisting a group of adult EFL students studying English as their BA major in a University in Iran to realize their latent potential in learning reading comprehension.

2. Review of Related Literature

2.1 Theoretical Background of the Study

DA is framed within Sociocultural Theory (SCT) of mind which was initially proposed by the Russian psychologist Vygotsky and includes two crucial concepts: mediation and the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD). SCT deals with the role of social context in learning and underscores the central role that social relationships and participation in culturally organized practices play in learning. In L2 research, SCT emphasizes the role that social interaction plays in learning and the nature of language as a communicative activity rather than as a formal linguistic system.

The central concept of Vygotsky's SCT is ZPD, which he introduced as a solution to overcome the instruction–assessment dualism. While trying to introduce the importance of Vygotsky's ZPD, Poehner (2011) stated that Vygotsky never used the term DA but his proposal of ZPD made his argument of instruction-assessment valid. According to Kozulin and Gindis (2007), the theoretical underpinning of DA is Vygotsky's often-quoted definition of ZPD as the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance, or in collaboration with more capable peers. For example, two 8 years old children may be able to complete a task that an average 8 years old can do. Next, more difficult tasks are presented with very little assistance from an adult. In the end, both children were able to complete the task. However, the styles methods they chose depended on how far they were willing to stretch their thinking process (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 86).

This means that there is a difference between learners' actual and proximal development and one can understand a learner's actual (current) level by exploring what he/she can do on his/her own, but only through exploring what the learner is able to do with a more-skilled other (e.g., parents, peers, teachers) is it possible to explore his/her potential development. Hence ZPD, based on Ableeva (2010), is comprised of the difference between what a learner can today do with the help of others (more skillful than him/her) and what he/she can do without help tomorrow.

Taking this into consideration, the present researchers attempted to help the at-risk advanced students participate in this study to be aware of their ZPD and try to activate and gain the best of their potential. De Beer (2006, p. 9) remarked that ZPD "reflects development itself", that is, "it is not only about what one is, but what one can become; it is not only what has developed but what is developing and can be viewed as a means to improve the testing of individual mental functioning."

The mere students' awareness of their potential to learn cannot guarantee their success because mediation is also effective in students' development. Mediation is the process by which other-regulated activities are transformed into self-regulated ones (Summers, 2008) and similar to ZPD, it is integral to DA (Ebadi & Saeedian, 2014, 2015, 2016a, 2016b) meaning that it is individual's potential development which is of importance in ZPD whereas mediation paves the way for the individual to achieve such development (Shrestha & Coffin, 2012). Development of mediation is also of high significance (Lantolf, 2000). For Vygotsky, development occurs if individuals move from other regulation to self-regulation. This type of development which was also underscored by Birjandi and Ebadi (2012) is referred to as microgenesis. Microgenetic development, based on Birjandi and Ebadi (p. 35) is actually "the development of mediation between expert and novice in a short time period."

2.2 Dynamic vs. NonDynamic (Static) Assessment

DA is a kind of assessment which is interactive. Lidz and Gindis (2003, p. 99) defined DA as "an approach to understanding individual differences and their implications for instruction that embeds intervention within testing procedure". Haywood and Lidz (2007, p. 1) defined DA as follows: "an interactive approach to conducting assessments within the domains of psychology, speech/language, or education that focuses on the ability of the learner to respond to intervention." Sternberg and Grigorenko (2002, p. vii), in a similar manner, discussed DA as a form of assessment that "takes into account the results of an intervention. In this intervention, the examiner teaches the examinee how to perform better on individual items or on the test as a whole." Though the last two definitions of DA provide a similar “conceptual footing of the current DA approaches" (Ableeva, 2010, p. 98), they fail to determine theoretical profiles of different approaches to DA. The directly drawn definition of DA based on the theoretical and methodological underpinnings of Vygotsky's SCT is as follows:

Dynamic assessment integrates assessment and instruction into a seam less, unified activity aimed at promoting learner development through appropriate forms of mediation that are sensitive to the individuals (or in some cases a group's) current abilities. In essence, DA is a procedure for simultaneously assessing and promoting development that takes account of the individual's (or group's) zone of proximal development (Lantolf & Poehner, 2004, p. 50).

This definition takes the ZPD, mediation, and development which are the central aspects of SCT into account and is a conceptual and theoretical basis for SCT-oriented DA.

According to Lidz and Gindis (2003), there are three main differences between dynamic and nondynamic procedures. They are as follows: the view of abilities, the purpose of assessment, and the role of the examiner. Lidz and Gindis (p. 100) believed that in DA, in contrary with NDA which is also called Static Assessment (SA), the abilities are not fixed and stable instead they can be changed because they are "the result of the individual's history of social interactions in the world". Regarding the purpose of conducting assessment, again DA and NDA are in contradictory. Because the purpose of assessment in DA is to diagnose the abilities that are fully matured and those that are still in the process of maturing. While in NDA or the same traditional methods of assessment only fully matured abilities and the product of development are reported. With regard to the third difference, namely, the role of the examiner, collaboration with examinee is crucial to development in DA in order to have a true picture of their abilities, but this is not the case in NDA and any aid from the examiners' side is to be avoided during the completion of assessment tasks. The last point has been synopsized by (Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2002, p. 29) who stated that the "conventional attitude of neutrality" characteristic of traditional assessments "is thus replaced by an atmosphere of teaching and helping."

To make a long story short, in DA the examinees’ final performance has been influenced by interaction with the mediator but NDA represents individuals’ solo performance and according to Lidz (1987, as cited in Birjandi & Ebadi, 2012) DA is at odds with NDA in that the former focuses on the learning processes and serves as a means of measuring the ZPD, but the latter focuses on already learned products.

2.3 Computerized Dynamic Assessment (C-DA)

Similar to DA, the central concept of the C-DA is grounded in Vygotsky's theoretical framework (1978) as well. Some studies have been conducted within DA scope while taking computer into very close considerations.

One of the studies conducted in the domain of C-DA was done by Jacobs (2001) in which preschoolers' potential language disorders were identified by a C-DA program called KIDTALK. The children were of culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds. Jacobs aimed to examine the effect of the C-DA training and scoring on the individual children's performance. In this program, a series of questions assess understanding of the language after the video is played. Whenever a learner answers incorrectly, the test pauses and the relevant portion of the introductory video is played again and the test resumes. If the leaner responds incorrectly again, the video is played for the final time. This means that each learner can have three attempts. Jacob's (2001) KIDTALK and Guthke and Beckmann's (2000) Lerntest have one thing in common. Instead of providing a graduated series of hints for learners who fail to answer correctly, they both allow only one form of mediation. In KIDTALK, the introductory video is replayed and in Lerntest, a tutorial module was used.

As another example, due to the large size of Freshman English classes in Taiwan and consequently the difficulty of providing human-to-human mediation to each individual learner, Teo (2012) conducted a study on examining 68 Taiwanese college EFL learners' inferential reading skills to show the effects of C-DA on promoting Taiwanese EFL college students’ metacognitive reading strategies in making inferences. To do so, he used the Viewlet Quiz 3 software to develop a C-DA program which lets the learners respond to the preprogrammed computerized intervention. Similar to other studies, its four levels of mediation in the C-DA procedure moves from implicit to explicit. That is, if a learner answers a question incorrectly, the computerized hints are presented to them in order of increasing explicitness.

The most recent research on the field of C-DA, to the best of the researchers' knowledge, was done by Teo (2014). She used an interventionist approach to DA by providing preprogrammed mediation during assessment for learners. Similar to his previous study, he conducted the study in a Taiwanese college and investigated the reading skills of 137 non-English major students, having divided them into a control and an experimental group. He investigated three reading skills (i.e., identifying main ideas or FMI), using contextual clues (CC), and making inferences (MI) using a C-DA program which provided the learners with preprogrammed mediation without having to rely on one-on-one mediation. The mediations provided in her C-DA program, similar to the ones used in this study, are consistent with the guidelines of Aljaafreh and Lantolf (1994) and range from the most implicit to the most explicit.

This study aimed to assess the degree the C-DA procedures could help individuals be cognizant of their latent abilities by offering prescripted mediation. To do so, the following question guided the study:

To what extent does computerized dynamic assessment help the participants realize their learning potential?

3. Methodology

Following most DA studies (Ableeva, 2010; Lantolf & Poehner, 2013; Poehner, 2005), this research uses qualitative methodology which best fits DA principles (Ableeva, 2010) but it can also be regarded as quantitative because it follows interventionist DA (Poehner & Lantolf, 2005). In other words, both qualitative and quantitative research have been used in the study.

3.1 Participants

The participants were selected from among all 47 undergraduates of Bachelor of Arts (BA) who studied Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL) at a university in Iran. Of these 47 students, 32 were selected non-randomly to take part in the study on availability basis. The mean age of the sample which was 27 years ranging from 26 to 33 indicated that the participants were adults. English was the second language of these adult participants, so this study was carried out in an L2 context. The homogeneity of the participants was taken for granted by claiming this statement (also being contended by Poehner, 2005) that the number of semesters the students have spent studying a language shows their proficiency level in that language. Of course, the results obtained from the DIALANG, a free online assessment system to determine learners’ proficiency level, were also indicative of the homogeneity of the participants. In addition to Poehner's claim, the researchers also made use of DIALANG to make sure about their status. Among the 32 participants, the results showed that 24 were at the B2 English reading comprehension level, seven were at the B1 proficiency level, and only one participant was at the C1 level.

An issue which is worth noting is the learners' real proficiency level. Because the researchers have taught many TEFL undergraduates who have not been able to speak English because of their very low proficiency, they decided to study them and see if it is possible to improve their progress or not. Having studied Teaching English as a Foreign Language (TEFL) for four years, some of these students were not even able to speak nonstop for some minutes and this means that, in addition to the DIALANG results, these students, in the very sense of the word, are 'at-risk' students and hence the reason for selecting them to take part in this study is clear. Of course, the educational system of this university, its professors, the students, their aim of studying, and so forth may also be responsible for these students' very low proficiency but for the aim of this research only the students themselves were investigated.

The importance of using this study lies in the contradiction between the terms 'advanced', and 'at-risk' learners which are representative of the 'should-be' level and ‘actual’ status of the participants of the present study respectively. Because the participants were seniors, they were considered as ‘advanced’ students but due to their low proficiency, based on the results obtained from the Placement Test of DIALANG which is the primary outcome of an EU funded project to deliver an instrument for aligning language learners on the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR), they were called 'at-risk' too. That is why these two contradictory terms have been used to refer to the participants of the present study. That the number of semesters can be indicative of the proficiency of students has also been given priority by Poehner (2005). Therefore, it is of really high importance to reiterate that the tests which have been used in this study were all suitable for advanced level students and that using DIALANG was just to reassure that students were 'at-risk'. In other words, the tests were not designed based on the results obtained from DIALANG but instead they were designed for advanced students by taking this for granted that the number of semesters indicates the proficiency of students.

3.2   Instrumentation

3.2.1 Computerized Dynamic Reading Test (CDRT)

To ensure whether C-DA could assist the learners realize their learning potential or not, the researchers utilized the previously validated and reliable software developed by Pishghadam and Barabadi (2012), namely, Computerized Dynamic Reading Test (CDRT). With regard to the software, it is worth mentioning that it can easily run on any PC given that the NET Framework software is installed on it. Students have to enter some information such as their name, age and major (students can choose a pseudonym to remain anonymous for other people but they should say it to the mediator) and after reading the software description go directly into the passage and answer the items while consulting the preplanned hints which are automatically shown if a wrong response is chosen. It takes about two hours to complete responding the test and after completing it a scoring file is created on the desktop to know about the test taker's performance.

3.2.2 DIALANG

DIALANG is an online assessment system which is free and is intended for individual adult language learners who aim to obtain diagnostic information about their linguistic proficiency for three of the four main skills, i.e. Reading, Listening and Writing and two subskills (i.e., Grammar and Vocabulary in fourteen languages). Having instructions and tests in all these languages has made DIALANG a practical way at identifying learners’ proficiency level. DIALANG's Assessment Framework and self-assessment statements are based on the Common European Framework of Reference (CEFR) for Languages; thus, it also gives feedback on the strengths and weaknesses of the learner's proficiency and advises about how to improve language skills. To determine the participants' proficiency level in this study, they were asked to visit the following address: http://dialangweb.lancaster.ac.uk. Because each test took about two hours for each individual to complete, the whole process took two days until the results of the whole participants' proficiency level were determined.

3.3 Data Collection Procedure

Upon the completion of the CDRT, the participants were reported of their performance which was saved in a scoring file on desktop to be analyzed later by the researchers. In addition to determining the total amount of time spent on responding the test, three more parts of information were provided as well: (a) individual student's first try of each item (NDA score), (b) Score gained with the use of hints (DA score), and (c) The number of hints used in each item.

Regarding the design of the study, the following stages were monitored: (a) the pretest; (b) the Enrichment Program; and (c) the posttest. The pretest, consisted of two passages which in addition to being similar to the texts used in the DIALANG with regard to the degree of difficulty, included items which assessed the same areas the participants showed to have problems with (e.g., their inability in connecting the ideas in the passages and their difficulty at identifying the main ideas of texts) as well. Having collected the pretest results and consequently having identified the participants’ problematic areas, the researchers determined the number of sessions to be held for the EP (two weeks: two sessions per week; each session one and a half hours). In the last stage of the design of this study, namely, the posttest, two scores were obtained through taking the results of the CDRT test as follows: actual or NDA score (i.e., without mediation or the first try of the participants) and mediated (DA) scores. This means that the CDRT which was developed by Pishghadam and Barabadi (2012) was used in the posttest design of this study. Similar to the pretest, a one-week period was determined to collect the data in this stage too because there were only seven computers available and the participants could not wait there for others to fulfill their job. In this stage which was done individually the students' score gained with the use of hints was their dynamic score and their score gained with no hint (i.e., their first try) was called their nondynamic score.

4. Data Analysis

In order to measure the students' potential for learning, Kozulin and Garb's (2002) formula which is called the Learning Potential Score (LPS) was used. Based on Poehner and Lantolf (2013), LPS shows how much mediation is likely required for an individual or a group of individuals to develop or move forward. Of course, through running the Pearson product moment correlation coefficient between nondynamic and gain scores, it is also possible to estimate the students' potential for learning. An interesting aspect of using Kozulin and Garb's (2002) formula lies in the way in which the outcomes of the DA procedure are reported. Instead of creating a qualitative report of each individual's performance for all stages of the study, a single score (or the LPS) is obtained through the difference between the learner's pretest and posttest scores which shows the learners' abilities. What follows is the Learning Potential Score (LPS) formula:

                       

Adopted from Kozulin and Garb (2002, p. 119)

(Where S post = dynamic scores; S pre = nondynamic scores; and S Max = the highest dynamic score gained in this test)

5. Results

The results showed that the participants' performance increased drastically in the posttest in comparison to the pretest with the only exception of participant 20 who gained only 2 points more than her pretest actual score. In other words, due to her great actual score (90) in the pretest, she was expected to accomplish her performance in the posttest, but she just improved it to 92 which indicates that she required no mediation. In general, including mediation in a test aims at representing the foregoing move of learners' performance, namely, their abilities might improve during the assessment course (Poehner & Lantolf, 2013). Though she showed little growth in the posttest, it was not surprising at all as in conjunction with Poehner et al.'s (2015) study she did not have adequate room for development within the test.

Table 1

Participants, Actual, and Mediated Scores, Gain Scores, and Learning Potential Scores (LPSs)

 

Students

Pretest

Posttest

 

Gain   Scores

 

LPS

Actual Scores*

Mediated Scores

Actual Scores

1.   Soma

20

63

35

46

1.17

2.   Soran

30

71

50

41

1.24

3.   Hemn

35

73

50

38

1.23

4.   Sheida

30

84

50

54

1.53

5.   Weisi

25

61

40

36

1.07

6.   Omid

30

73

55

43

1.28

7.   Shahin

55

68

50

13

0.9

8.   Maryam

30

66

50

36

1.13

9.   Bayan

30

59

45

29

0.97

10.   Nashmil

30

68

50

38

1.17

11.   Golaleh

35

65

40

30

1.05

12.   Roghayeh

45

78

60

33

1.23

13.   Haraleh

35

84

55

49

1.47

14.   Hataw

40

69

50

29

1.08

15.Mohammad

30

70

50

40

1.22

16.   Somayeh

10

58

40

48

1.17

17.Asmar

30

73

50

43

1.28

18.   Amin

30

78

35

48

1.4

19.   Rojyar

20

54

15

34

0.97

20.   Farzaneh

90

92

80

2

1.04

21.   Kosar

20

68

45

48

1.28

22.   Nabiz

20

63

35

43

1.17

23.   Ata

35

58

30

23

0.9

24.   Tahsin

40

66

50

26

1.02

25.   Farasat

45

62

45

17

0.87

26.   Mosleh

20

86

55

66

1.68

27.   Diman

35

54

25

19

0.81

28.   Omar

30

63

40

33

1.06

29.   Hadi

30

57

35

27

0.93

30.   Aram

30

66

40

36

1.13

31.   Sarah

40

78

55

38

1.28

32.   Nasrin

35

80

55

45

1.38

* Maximum Score = 90

Similar to Poehner et al. (2015, p. 12) who asserted that "learners with higher actual scores generally also had higher mediated scores, (emphasis is mine)", the high achievers in the pretest also had high mediated scores but their gain scores were not as high the others. For instance, the second highest scorer in the pretest (participant 7) was among the low performers in the posttest as she gained only 13 points. Plus, participant 20 who gained the highest score in the pretest did not gain as well as she was supposed to. Therefore, in line with Poehner et al. (2015, p. 12) it can be stated that outperforming a participant in the pretest does not necessarily (or generally as they say) imply that his/her performance will be higher than that of the others in the posttest. In this regard, the participants who outperformed others (participants 7 & 20) in the pretest were taken into account. Though they scored 55 and 90 respectively in the pretest (i.e., their performance was actually better than their counterparts', their mediated scores were 68 and 92 respectively). This is indicative of the fact that simply one's better performance in the pretest does not imply his/her superiority in the posttest too. For example, the lowest number in the posttest, 54, belonged to participant 27 who scored 35 in the pretest and gained only 19 points upon reception of mediation.

Table 1 provides the individual participants' information on both pre- and posttest actual scores, posttest mediated scores, gain scores and ultimately LPS. The actual score in the posttest is what the students would have gotten in a traditional test where only the first responses counted, but the mediated score is the weighted score that includes points earned for attempts beyond the first. This means that the main actual score to be included in achieving LPS and gain scores is the actual score of the pretest not the posttest. With regard to the mediated scores, it should be stated that they are not in themselves reflective of learning but instead they are indicative of learners' differential responsiveness to mediation which is the mechanism that learning is dependent on (Poehner & Lantolf, 2013). The information is based on the participants' pretest and their scoring files of the CDRT in the posttest. In order to build upon the above short descriptions of the participants' performance, the research question is answered as follows:

As Table 1 shows there is an improvement in the participants' posttest scores in comparison to that of their pretest. This indicates the effectiveness of the C-DA program on the participants in that it helped them recognize that they could ameliorate. Though Table 1 in itself can be representative of this fact, the researchers measured the participants' LPS through adopting the formula created by Kozulin and Garb (2002).

Before going any further, mentioning some notes is of significance. First, due to the high number of participants it would be so disorganizing and untidy to include the LPS of all of them in just one Figure. Hence, they are shown in two Figures; each containing one 16-participant group that were already divided in the EP sessions due to lack of a spacious class. Therefore, Figure 1 deals with the first 16 participants and the second one is about the others. As it was already mentioned they were not included in any of the groups based on any purposive plans, (i.e., the only criterion for inclusion in a group was their personal requests such as the time when they were free and could be in the institute). Second, to keep up the same format to refer to the participants throughout the study and also because of convenience, only their number (e.g., participant 1, participant 2, or the 1st participant, the 2nd participant, …) has been used and the participants’ names (pseudonym in all cases) which are written in Table 1 are not used. Third, the aim of using the maximum actual score at calculating LPS is both to quantify the degree of improvement and not to penalize the highest actual scorers as well (Poehner et al., 2015). And finally, Table 1 containing the participants' pre- and posttest scores along with their LPS has been also consulted to gain more insights from their performance.

By applying the above formula which provides a basis for differentiating between low and high learning potential students (Kozulin & Garb, 2002), the researchers measured the participants' LPS. As shown in Table 1 and illustrated in Figures 1 and 2, the participants all showed to have high LPS based on Kozulin and Garb (2002) who asserted that in case LPS≥1.0, then that student has a high learning potential, if a student's LPS is between 0.71–0.8, they have mid LPS and if it is less than 0.71 (LPS≤0.71), that student has low LPS; proposing that each student's requirement of instructional support varies depending on that student's LPS.

Figure 1.The participants' pre- and posttest scores along with their LPS

The range is 0 to 100 in pre- and posttest.

The range is 0 to 2 in the LPS.

Figure 1 shows the data about the first 16 participants' pre- and posttest scores which is indicative of progress in their performance. Because relying on students' actual (pretest) scores has nothing to tell us about their ZPD (Poehner et al., 2015), the mediated (posttest) scores were used to provide the researchers with the range of the participants' capabilities. In addition to the scores, the participants' LPSs were used to reassure about their progress and learning potential. As shown in Table 1 and Figures 1 and 2, their LPS scores range from 0.81 to 1.68; a point which indicates their high LPS (Kozulin & Garb, 2002).

Figure 2. The participants' pre- and posttest scores along with their LPS

The range is 0 to 100 in pre- and posttest.

The range is 0 to 2 in the LPS.

What makes us draw attention closely to in Figure 2 is the 26th participant who unbelievably has a high LPS. His LPS is 1.68 which shows the first highest LPS amongst the others and buttresses the effectiveness of the intervention provided which unfolded their hidden potential being always discarded by traditional testing that assumes future similar to the present (Poehner, 2007). His high mediated score, based on Poehner and Lantolf (2013, p. 335) is also "an indication that the learner responded more favorably to mediation."

The least pretest score (10) belonged to participant 16 who achieved an incredible gain score of 48 in the posttest yielding a high LPS of 1.17 which is identical to participants 1, 10, and 22 whose actual scores were 20, 30, and 20 anew respectively but ameliorated their performance by gaining 43, 38 and 43 scores upon reception of mediation respectively. This proves that in case students are provided with sufficient and ZPD-based mediation, they can enhance their performance (Tajeddin & Tayebipour, 2012). In the same vein, in addition to participants 1 and 22, three more (participants 19, 21, and 26) scored 20 in the pretest which was the second least score amongst all. Their mediated or (DA) posttest scores were 54, 68, and 86 respectively which represents their high LPSs as well.

However, this is not true about all of the other participants. For instance, participants 20 and 7 whose actual scores were the first two top scores (90 and 55 respectively) gained no more points than 2 and 13 under mediation and yielded their LPSs to be 1.04 and 0.9. As in Poehner et al. (2015) and Poehner and Lantolf (2013), the results of this study show that those who performed less well in the pretest have taken more advantages from the mediation presented in the EP sessions in comparison to those who outperformed others. This point was supported by Poehner et al. (2015) who referred to the existence of less room for improvement in learners with high actual scores in the pretest.

In this study, in concomitant with Poehner et al. (2015), Teo (2012) and Mardani and Tavakoli (2011), it can be stated that learners with the same pretest scores might turn out to have different or even drastically different DA posttest scores and hence different LPSs. For instance, consulting Table 1, one can assure that those with the same actual score (20) in the pretest (1, 19, 21, 22, and 26) showed differing mediated scores except in the cases of 1 and 22 which was elaborated earlier. This point depicts their high LPSs after receiving mediation. The same is true about the ones whose actual score was identical, the only difference was that their mediated scores were not the same. That is, they all progressed significantly but their scores, except in some cases such as participants 4, 13, 26, and 32, are not drastically different from each other. Mardani and Tavakoli (2011, p. 691), similarly, stated that "it is confirmed that students with a similar performance level demonstrate different, and in some cases dramatically different ability to learn and use new text comprehension strategies." A point which has frequently been confirmed in DA research by the above-mentioned researchers as well.

5. Discussion and Pedagogical Implications

This study was carried out to examine at-risk students' reading comprehension in the context of L2 through C-DA as an overall purpose. In terms of the main aim of the study (i.e., the individual learners' learning potential, it became more apparent that simply earning a same score by two or more individuals in pretest does not make for their same performance level in other stages). For instance, consulting Table 1 anew, one can assure that those with the same actual score (20) in the pretest (1, 19, 21, 22, & 26) showed differing mediated scores except in the cases of 1 and 22 which were earlier elaborated in the preceding chapter. This point, which aligns with the findings of studies, such as Poehner et al. (2015), Teo (2012), Kozulin (2011), and Mardani and Tavakoli (2011), depicts the learners' high LPSs after receiving mediation. Concomitantly, it shed some more light to the argument indicating the insufficiency of standard assessment procedures in estimating individuals' learning potential and their other prominent drawback of not providing enough feedback for teachers to identify the conditions in which leaners can progress (Kozulin & Garb, 2002). In other words, the obtained results from the LPS generated by the C-DA procedure approved that there is a concordance between any leaner's learning potential and his/her score; this paved the way for the researchers to involve them in conditions contributive of their development concerning reading comprehension.

Like any other studies this study had some shortcomings as well which beg for further exploration. The study only involved a group of 32 advanced at-risk Iranian students regardless of taking their gender, age, learning styles, so on into consideration. Although they could simply be measured and included in the results of the study, the aims were far beyond these factors. Other sources of mediation provision such as novice-expert and peer-peer interactions and other human or physical tools can be controlled to gain a better interpretation of the results. Anyway, these variables can be explored to see whether a more comprehensive picture of the impacts of the C-DA procedure on individuals' learning can be gained.

There are some pedagogical implications drawn from the study that are worth noting. The first implication is taking low- and nogainers into account. In total contradiction with traditional or NDA forms of assessment, the DA procedure in general and the C-DA one in particular promote individuals to achieve success by being provided with ZPD-attuned mediation. Though some issues such as fairness in education and its accessibility might be interruptive for the DA owing to not having enough experts in one specific research domain (Poehner, 2011), here at-risk learners were meticulously explored to observe fairness in a remote border city in Iran. In concomitant with Lantolf and Poehner (2013), the individuals' LPSs should not be used to endow some with provision of language learning opportunities while discarding or denying others from the same opportunities. It is suggested as well that their LPSs instead be regarded quite relatively as results of placement tests (i.e., learners should receive a type of instruction that is corresponding to their ZPD not their ZAD). That is to say that only few participants (e.g., participant 19) would be allowed to be instructed in case NDA procedures or pretest scores were taken into account. This is in agreement with Antón (2009) who concluded that teachers might end with misrepresentations of individuals' learning abilities provided that they only make use of traditional test results.

In conclusion, the C-DA principles helped the researchers identify the learners' potentiality in reading comprehension. Actually, this is what DA seeks for. According to Feuerstein et al. (2000), the most appealing point which distinguishes DA from other ways of assessment is, without a doubt, its optimistic view towards human beings (regardless of having primary or secondary disabilities, based on Gindis, 2003); it strongly believes in their development in case of being provided with more ZPD-oriented tasks, even if to a small extent. As just mentioned, Gindis (2003) reported from Vygotsky that primary disabilities are the same biological problems such as visual or auditory ones, whereas secondary disabilities are caused by the manner people in a particular society react to primary disabilities, namely, their expectations, culture, attitudes towards disable (mentally or physical) individuals greatly influence on providing the opportunities for them to whether take part in activities with their friends or not. Even nowadays, in different cultures disable people are neglected to a great extent, but thanks to DA (C-DA) principles they are granted to have access to opportunities whose taking might result in overcoming some of the challenges they have always coped with.

Ableeva, R. (2008). The effects of dynamic assessment on L2 listening comprehension. In J. P. Lantolf & M. E. Poehner (Eds.), sociocultural theory and the teaching of second languages (pp. 112-134). London: Equinox.
Ableeva, R. (2010). Dynamic assessment of listening comprehension in second language learning (Unpublished Doctoral dissertation). Pennsylvania State University, Pennsylvania, the USA.
Aljaafreh, A., & Lantolf, J. P. (1994). Negative feedback as regulation and second language learning in the zone of proximal development. The Modern Language Journal, 78, 465-483.
Antón, M. (2009). Dynamic assessment of advanced second language learners. Foreign Language Annals, 42(3), 576-598.
Birjandi, P., & Ebadi, S. (2012). Microgenesis in dynamic assessment of L2 learners’ socio-cognitive development via web 2.0. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences32, 34-39.
Birjandi, P., Estaji, M., & Deyhim, T. (2013). The impact of dynamic assessment on reading comprehension and metacognitive awareness of reading strategy use in Iranian high school learners. Iranian Journal of Language Testing, 3(2), 60-77.
Carney, J. J., & Cioffi, G. (1992). The dynamic assessment of reading abilities. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 39(2), 107-114.
De Beer, M. (2006). Dynamic testing: Practical solutions to some concerns. SA Journal of Industrial Psychology32(4), 8-14.
Dole, J., Brown, K., & Trathen, W. (1996). The effects of strategy instruction on the comprehension performance of at-risk students. Reading Research Quarterly, 31(1), 62-88.
Dörfler, T., Golke, S., & Artelt, C. (2009). Dynamic assessment and its potential for the assessment of reading competence. Studies in Educational Evaluation35(2), 77-82.
Ebadi, S. (2014). L2 private speech in online dynamic assessment: a sociocultural perspective. Iranian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 17(1), 49-70.
Ebadi, S., & Saeedian, A. (2014). The role of l1 in online l2 dynamic assessment mediation. The Iranian EFL Journal, 13(1), 96-120.
Ebadi, S., & Saeedian, A. (2015). The effects of computerized dynamic assessment on promoting at-risk advanced Iranian EFL students' reading skills. Issues in Language Teaching, 4(2), 26-1.
Ebadi, S., & Saeedian, A. (2016a). Exploring transcendence in EFL learners' reading comprehension through computerized dynamic assessment. Iranian Journal of Language Teaching Research, 4(1), 27-45.
Ebadi, S., & Saeedian, A. (2016b). Planning future instructional programs through computerized L2 dynamic assessment. Teaching English with Technology, 16(4), 12-32.
Feuerstein, R., Feuerstein, R. S., & Falik, L. H. (2010). Beyond smarter: Mediated learning and the brain's capacity for change. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Feuerstein, R., Y. Rand, and J. E. Rynders. (2000). Don't accept me as I am. Helping retarded performers excel. New York: Plenum.
Gindis, B. (2003). Remediation through education: sociocultural theory and children with special needs. In A. Kozulin, B. Gindis, V. S. Ageyev, S. M. Miller (Eds.). Vygotsky's Educational Theory in Cultural Context (pp. 200-221). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
 
 
Guthke, J., & Beckmann, J. F. (2000). The learning test concept and its applications in practice. In C. S. Lidz & J. G. Elliott (Eds.), Dynamic assessment: prevailing models and applications (pp.17–69). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
Haywood, H. C., & Lidz, C. S. (2007). Dynamic assessment in practice: Clinical and educational applications. Cambridge University Press.
Haywood, H., & Tzuriel, D. (2002). Applications and challenges in dynamic assessment. Peabody Journal of Education, 77(2), 40–63.
Jacobs, E.L. (2001). The effects of adding dynamic assessment components to a computerized preschool language screening test. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 22 (4), 217–226.
Kozulin, A., & Gindis, B. (2007). Sociocultural theory and education of children with special needs, from defectology to remedial pedagogy. In H. Daniels, M. Cole, & J. V. Wertsch (Eds.), The Cambridge companion to Vygotsky (pp. 332-336).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kozulin, A., & Garb, E. (2002). Dynamic assessment of EFL text comprehension of at-risk students. School Psychology International23(1), 112-127.
Mardani, M., & Tavakoli, M. (2011). Beyond reading comprehension: The effect of adding a dynamic assessment component on EFL reading comprehension. Journal of Language Teaching and Research2(3), 688-696.
Lantolf, J. P. (2000). Second language learning as a mediated process. Language Teaching, 33(2), 79-96.
Lantolf, J. P., & Poehner, M. E. (2004). Dynamic assessment of L2 development: bringing the past into the future. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 1(2), 49-72.
Lantolf, J. P., & Poehner, M. E. (2013). The unfairness of equal treatment: objectivity in L2 testing and dynamic assessment. Educational Research and Evaluation19(2-3), 141-157.
Lantolf, J. P., & Thorne, S. L. (2006). Sociocultural theory and the genesis of second language development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lidz, C. S., & Gindis, B. (2003). Dynamic assessment of the evolving cognitive functions in children. In A. Kozulin, B. Gindis, V. S. Ageyev, S. M. Miller (Eds.), Vygotsky's Educational Theory in Cultural Context (pp. 99-116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nassaji, H., & Swain, M. (2000). A Vygotskian perspective on corrective feedback in L2: The effect of random versus negotiated help on the learning of English articles. Language Awareness9(1), 34-51.
Pishghadam, R., Barabadi, E. (2012). Constructing and validating computerized assessment of l2 reading comprehension. Iranian Journal of Applied Linguistics15(1), 73-95.
Pishghadam, R., Barabadi, E., & Kamrood, A. M. (2011). The differing effect of computerized dynamic assessment of L2 reading comprehension on high and low achievers. Journal of Language Teaching & Research, 2(6), 1353-1358.
Poehner, M. E. (2005). Dynamic assessment of oral proficiency among advanced L2 learners of French (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Pennsylvania State University, University Park.
Poehner, M, E. (2008). Dynamic assessment: A Vygotskian approach to understanding and promoting L2 development. New York: Springer.
Poehner, M. E. (2011). Dynamic assessment: Fairness through the prism of mediation. Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice, 18(2), 99-112.
Poehner, M. E., & Lantolf, J. P. (2005). Dynamic assessment in the language classroom. Language Teaching Research, 9, 233-265.
Poehner, M. E., & Lantolf, J. P. (2013). Bringing the ZPD into the equation: Capturing L2 development during computerized dynamic assessment (C-DA). Language Teaching Research, 17(3), 1-21.
Poehner, M. E., Zhang, J., & Lu, X. (2015). Computerized dynamic assessment (C-DA): Diagnosing L2 development according to learner responsiveness to mediation. Language testing, 32(3), 337-357.
Shrestha, P., & Coffin, C. (2012). Dynamic assessment, tutor mediation and academic writing development. Assessing Writing, 17(1), 55-70.
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2002). Dynamic testing. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Summers, R. (2008). Toward an online model of dialogic engagement. In J. P. Lantolf and M. E. Poehner (Eds.), sociocultural theory and the teaching of second languages (pp. 91-112). London: Equinox.
Tajeddin, Z., & Tayebipour, F. (2012). The effect of dynamic assessment on EFL learners' acquisition of request and apology. The Journal of Teaching Language Skills, 4(2), 87-118.
Teo, A. (2012). Effects of dynamic assessment on college EFL learners' reading skills. The Journal of Asia TEFL, 9(1), 57-94.
Teo, A. (2014). Beyond traditional testing: Exploring the use of computerized dynamic assessment to improve EFL learners' reading. Arab World English Journal, 5(1), 42-58.
Tzuriel, D., & Shamir, A. (2002). The effects of mediation in computer assisted dynamic assessment. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 18(1), 21–32.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher mental processes. MA: Harvard University.