Task-based Collaborative Interaction in a CMC Environment: A Form-Focused Perspective

Document Type : Original Article

Authors

Shahrekord University, Iran

Abstract

 
The study explored the effect of task-based collaborative interactions in a Synchronous Computer-mediated Communication (SCMC) environment on English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners' development of grammatical knowledge and accuracy of a specified structure, that is, conditional clauses. To this end, at first, two intact EFL-grammar classes from an Iranian university were randomly assigned to control and experimental groups. A grammar test focusing on the above-mentioned structure was used at the pretest and posttest times. The control group learners were exposed to mainstream noncollaborative instruction in which they performed a number of tasks individually within the context of a teacher-fronted classroom. The learners of the experimental group, however, carried out the same planned tasks through peer-peer and teacher-student collaborative interactions in the form of text chats in an SCMC environment (i.e., Skype). Secondly, to trace learners' trajectories of grammatical accuracy, their L2 written outputs produced during the task performance were analyzed, employing the error-free T-unit ratio. The SCMC participants were also interviewed to elicit their attitudes towards employing the approach to grammar instruction. As to grammatical knowledge test, ANCOVA results revealed that the experimental group outperformed the control group in the posttest. In terms of grammatical accuracy, subsequent t-test results indicated significant gains for the SCMC group. Furthermore, learner interviews indicated that most learners had generally positive attitudes toward CMC-oriented grammar instruction. The findings suggested that collaborative interactions in form of text-based exchanges in CMC learning environments can be a useful platform for L2 grammar instruction and learning.

Keywords


1. Introduction

Since the advent of (social) constructivism as an interesting learning theory within the field of second and foreign language (L2) education, the concept of collaboration has received considerable attention from many researchers and educators. According to Kumar (1996), collaborative learning involves instructional methods that try to facilitate the learning process through collaborative efforts among leaners who work on a given learning task. In essence, it provides an environment in which a learner interacts with his or her collaborative peers to solve a specified problem. Therefore, it can be perceived that peer-peer collaborative interaction is of considerable significance not only in collaborative learning, when viewed from a narrow perspective, but, as Swain (2000) states, in L2 learning process from a broader point of view.

Nonetheless, one of the greatest challenges, which the educators of English as a foreign language (EFL) have to tackle, is how to provide opportunities for peer-peer interactions in the target language and for meaningful purposes. It becomes even more difficult when the instructors also attempt to have learners notice linguistic forms and constructions. In other words, as Meskill and Anthony (2005) state, the practical limitations of teacher-fronted classrooms prevent teachers from providing learners with adequate instructional supports at appropriate moments and engaging them in collaborative interaction while carefully raising their awareness of language forms and structures. 

With the emergence of Internet in recent years, there has been a growing interest in integrating computer-mediated communication (CMC) technology into the field of second language acquisition (SLA). CMC refers to any communication which is maintained through the employment of two or more electronic devices (McQuail, 2005). It involves a wide variety of online tools such as chats, discussion boards, course management systems, social networking sites, and virtual realities which can be used synchronously or asynchronously. These two CMC modes (i.e., synchronous & asynchronous) contain various numbers of modules which, according to AbuSeileek and Qatawneh (2013), can help instructors and administrators in providing ample opportunities for communication among language learners. In fact, the various available affordances of CMC environments, as Abrams (2008) mentions, set the scene for learners to interact, negotiate, and collaborate with each other and, therefore, develop their communicative competence. In addition, the written nature of the dialogues and the interactions carried out in CMC offers learners a "greater opportunity to attend to and reflect on the form and content of the communication" (Kern & Warschauer, 2000, p. 15). This characteristic of CMC, thus, serves a considerable advantage to L2 instructors, helping them attain their goal of attracting learners' attention to L2 forms.

In the same vein, Task-based Language Teaching (TBLT) has been undeniably well recognized by many scholars as one of the most effective and successful approaches to language teaching (e.g., Ellis, 2003; Nunan, 2004). In SLA research, tasks have been widely employed to trigger language production, interaction, collaboration, and focus on form which are all admitted to facilitate L2 learning (Van den Branden, 2006).

In order to further investigate the task-technology interface within SLA, termed as 'elective affinities' by Ortega (2009), the study, therefore, aims at exploring the possible effects of task-based collaborative interactions and dialogues in a synchronous CMC (SCMC) environment on improving EFL learners' grammatical knowledge of a specific structure, namely, conditional clauses. To this purpose, EFL learners were encouraged to carry out a number of tasks collaboratively through text chats in an SCMC environment via Skype. They were encouraged to employ various existing affordances of the environment, such as interactional and group conferencing modules, the capability of sending and receiving feedbacks both among themselves and between the learners and the instructor, and screen sharing. Their grammatical knowledge of the specified structure was, then, compared with that of the learners who were exposed to a noncollaborative task-based approach in a teacher-fronted context. In addition, learners’ probable grammatical ability development with conditional clauses was measured and compared in the two groups mainly by analyzing their L2 written outputs and through a well-known developmental measure of grammatical accuracy, that is, error-free T-unit ratio. Finally, the participants of the SCMC group were interviewed after the treatment in order to express their opinion on employing a task-based collaborative approach to grammar instruction in an SCMC environment.

2. Theoretical Background

2.1 Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning

As a reaction against the development of a software application that promoted an isolated noncollaborative learning environment, Computer-supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL) became increasingly popular in the last decade of the 20th century (Stahl, Koschmann, & Surthers, 2006). CSCL is a burgeoning branch of learning sciences, mainly dealing with how people can learn together with the mediation of computers. Although the concept has a seemingly simple definition, as Stahl et al. (2006) state, it conceals considerable complexity. In fact, the inclusion of such variables as collaboration and computer mediation into any learning processes can make the situation problematic enough, making it even more difficult to apply it to the SLA process.

According to Hsiao (1996), SCCL assumes that group processes and group dynamics can be supported and facilitated through computer-mediated systems and environments, a fact which a face-to-face traditional communication cannot achieve, even though the latter is not to be substituted by this emerging concept. These environments enable users to communicate their ideas and information with other users, to access information and documents, and to give and receive feedback on collaborative problem-solving activities.

There are various types of CSCL environments, and they may include 'groupware' which are particularly developed for CSCL, various CMC tools which are employed for collaborative purposes, or a set of CMC tools simultaneously used in a collaborative objective (Lehtinen, Hakkarainen, Lipponen, Rahikainen, & Muukkonen, 1999). The current study employed an SCMC environment via Skype to trigger task-based collaborative interactions among EFL learners themselves and between the learners and the instructor.

2.2 Peer-Peer Interaction, CMC, and Sociocultural Theory

Generally, CMC-based studies, as Cheon (2008) states, have been shaped within the framework of the Interactionist theory. Recently, though, due to suggestions from a number of scholars (e.g., Chapelle, 2001; Kern & Warschauer, 2000) to approach CMC from a sociocognitive perspective, there have been attempts to carry out CMC-oriented research inspired by sociocultural-theory premises (Lee, 2004; O'Rourke, 2005; Thorne, 2003).

Developed mainly from Vygotsky's (1978) concepts, the sociocultural theory underscores the role of activity as the milestone of human learning and development which is defined as any purposeful behavior being facilitated through employing various tools among which language, as the semiotic system controlling the human intellect, is assumed to be the most important one. In fact, from Vygotsky’s point of view, any social interaction which is mediated by language leads to the development of human's higher-order functions. In other words, human cognition is shaped through social activity. Based on this view, social interaction plays an important role in providing a situation in which one can "learn language, learn about language, and learn through language" (Warschauer, 1997, p. 471).

Another important concept within sociocultural theory is Vygotsky's (1978) Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD). What he meant by ZPD can be defined as the gap between a child's "actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving" and the higher level of "potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers" (p. 86). Learning within ZPD, as Aljaafreh and Lantolf (1994) mention, is usually assumed to be based on expert-novice interaction in which an expert (i.e., a parent or a teacher) assists a novice (i.e., a child or a learner) incessantly and contingently. Recently, nonetheless, there has been growing interest (e.g., Foster & Ohta, 2005; Ohta, 2000; Storch, 2002) in extending the scope of ZPD to peer-peer interaction in SLA. According to Lantolf (2000), ZPD is "more appropriately conceived as the collaborative constructions of opportunities for individuals to develop their abilities" (p. 17). This was also properly indicated by Vygotsky himself where he emphasized the role of collaborative learning in helping learners develop through their ZPD. Overall, the sociocultural approach stresses the collaborative effort for the coconstruction of knowledge and meaning in an interpersonal and interactive environment, a fact which highlights the importance of dialogic and dynamic nature of peer-peer interaction.

As far as L2 learning is concerned, it is of outmost importance, therefore, to create interactive learning environments in which learners can socialize and interact with each other easily and, consequently, construct their L2 knowledge collaboratively and through peer assistance. Various affordances of CMC, among other things, can fulfill the need for increasing opportunities for collaborative interaction among language learners. In fact, the unique characteristics of CMC provide L2 learners with great chances of having rich negotiated interaction, effective collaboration, and joint construction of L2 forms, all of which together can enhance various aspects of learners' L2 knowledge and ability.

2.3 Form-Focused Instruction and CMC

Grammar is conceived as an integral part of any languages without which learning a new language is almost impossible. However, it is considered as one of the most controversial issues within the field of language teaching. The way grammar should be taught to language learners, whether implicitly or explicitly, has been the locus of great controversy (Ellis, 2001). Looking back through the past century, one can get a better grasp of the very fact that along with all ups and downs of different language teaching methods popularity, the position conceived of grammar and the way it should be instructed have been constantly changing.

Grammar instruction, as Richards and Reppen (2014) state, primarily centers on distinguishing between grammatical knowledge and grammatical ability. Grammatical knowledge involves the knowledge of rules which identify the grammaticality of a language. It is an idiosyncratic feature of traditional approaches to grammar instruction in which "learners are provided with opportunities for repetition of the target feature and are expected to perform the grammatical feature correctly" (Ellis, 2003, p. 168). On the other hand, grammatical ability, as Jones (2012) puts it, accounts for learners' ability in using grammar to help them communicate in various spoken and written discourse and, therefore, calls for a different pedagogical approach. This ability enables learners to make accurate grammatical choices in different communicative events such as having an online chat with a friend, writing an e-mail to a colleague, or conversing with someone.

As a result, and in order to facilitate the development of grammatical ability in L2 learners, language instructors should provide learners with suitable opportunities to interact with one another easily, to exchange genuine information, and to perform the activities which give them the pleasure of fulfilling meaningful and authentic purposes while at the same time focusing on language forms (Larsen-Freeman, 2011).

The burgeoning area of CMC technology, as stated earlier, seems promising for achieving the above-mentioned aim. CMC can create unique learning conditions for L2 learners and present them with communicative opportunities together with useful affordances that were once unimaginable and well beyond their reach. It is suggested that, as a result of such increased opportunities for interaction, the learners be forced to retrieve appropriate forms and functions from their cognitive repository and, therefore, produce more language output which, according to Swain's (1985) Output Hypothesis, 'is a necessary mechanism of acquisition' (p. 252). The role of comprehensible output, according to Swain, is "to provide opportunities for contextualized, meaningful use, to test out hypothesis about the target language, and to move the learner from a purely semantic analysis of the language to a syntactic analysis of it" (p. 252).

Recently, there have been a growing number of studies investigating the efficiency of CMC tools in providing more collaborative learning environments, enhancing learners' interactions, increasing language output, and drawing L2 learners' conscious attention to language forms, all of which seem to promote and facilitate L2 learning. Significant among them are the studies carried out by Appel and Mullen (2000), Kötter (2002), and O'Rourke (2005) which demonstrated that employing collaborative e-mail exchanges and MOOs (object-oriented multiuser domains) affordances were influential in drawing learners' attention to language forms.

In another study carried out by Kelm (1992), the possible role of CMC in an L2 teaching situation was examined. The preliminary results suggested that computer-mediated discussion sessions may increase learners' participation, reduce their anxiety during the conversation, allow learners to speak without being interrupted, give them opportunities to identify their language errors personally, and finally enhance interlanguage communication and interaction among L2 learners.

Moreover, Nagata (1996) compared the effect of a computer-mediated instruction with a traditional workbook one on enhancing L2 Japanese learners' morphosyntactic knowledge. The results of the study revealed that the computer-mediated instruction was more influential than the other intervention in learners' internalizing L2 Japanese particles and structures.

Nagata's (1998a, 1998b) following studies also confirmed the findings of her previous one. Nagata (1998a), for instance, examined the efficiency of the two computer-mediated instructional programs, an input-focused and an output-oriented one, on enhancing L2 learners’ knowledge of Japanese honorifics by comparing the performance of two group learners in an achievement, a retention, and an oral test. Nagata (1998b) focused mainly on L2 learners' internalization of Japanese nominal modifiers through the two above-mentioned interventions. The results of both studies revealed that computer-mediated instruction was influential in recognition of the target forms. Nonetheless, the learners in output-based group outperformed the ones in input-based group in the production of the two mentioned structures.

However, the current literature also contains some studies whose findings are not very encouraging. Whereas the findings of some studies report participants’ reduced grammatical accuracy (e.g., Kern, 1995) during computer-mediated instruction, others either suggest the ineffectiveness of CMC in enhancing SLA (e.g., Loewen & Erlam, 2006) or indicate no differences between the grammatical knowledge of the learners being exposed to traditional instruction and the ones who received a hybrid intervention, that is a mixture of class meetings and online computer-mediated instruction (e.g., Chenoweth & Murday, 2003).

Further research is, therefore, required to shed more light on the issue and attempt to bridge the existing gap.

3. Purpose of the Study

The results of earlier research have confirmed the positive effect of CMC on peer-peer interaction (e.g., Blake, 2000; Smith, 2003, 2005). Most of the related studies have focused on the L2 learners' meaning negotiation and its effect on developing L2 learners' communicative competence during the interactions (e.g., Lee, 2002; Pellettieri, 2000). Little attention, though, is paid to learners' collaborative efforts, dialogues, and knowledge building in CMC-oriented environments. Even fewer studies have examined this issue in EFL contexts. Due to the significant role of computer technology and especially CMC in SLA and considering the potentials of TBLT for setting the scene for learners’ collaboration and interaction, the study intends to identify the role of CMC in improving EFL learners' grammatical knowledge and ability by exposing the learners to CMC-oriented task-based collaborative interactions and comparing their performances with those who were instructed through a task-based noncollaborative approach in a teacher-fronted context. In particular, the study addressed the following research questions:

1. What is the effect of task-oriented collaborative instruction in an SCMC environment on EFL learners' development of grammatical knowledge and grammatical accuracy?

2. What are Iranian EFL learners' attitudes towards employing a task-based CMC-oriented collaborative approach to grammar instruction?

4. Methodology

4.1 Participants

The study employed a cohort of 120 undergraduate EFL students from two Iranian universities for the test-construction and instruction phases. They were both male (27.5%) and female (72.5%) freshmen ranging in age from 19 to 23 and majoring in English Literature and Translation. Having had passed a course in English Grammar (I), they now registered for the course in Grammar (II). From among the whole participants, 60 students from a university in center of Iran, namely, University of Isfahan, took part in different stages of test construction and validation, and the remaining 60 ones from a university in southwest of Iran (i.e., Shahrekord University) constituted the main participants of the current study. In fact, due to institutional constraints, random selection was not possible. However, two intact classes, each consisting of 30 male and female learners, were randomly assigned to experimental (SCMC) and control groups. Although the participants were informed that they were participating in a study, they did not know about the instructional differences involved. The participants fully agreed to take part in the study with anonymous identities.

4.2 Instrumentation

Four types of instruments were employed in the study as follows: a) a general language proficiency test, b) a researcher-made grammar test, c) a developmental ratio measure of accuracy for gauging the participants’ grammatical ability, and d) an open-ended question for interview. The collected data from the last instrument together with the data obtained from the same participants' written diaries during the treatment were examined to answer the last research question of the current study, that is, the participants’ attitudes on employing task-based CMC-oriented collaborative interaction to grammar instruction.

4.2.1 The proficiency test

In order to ensure the homogeneity of the participants in terms of language proficiency before the treatment and, also, to estimate the concurrent validity of the researcher-made grammar test, a language proficiency test was employed in the study. It involved a
paper-based test of English as a foreign language (TOEFL), originally constructed and standardized by the Educational Testing Service in 2004. The test consisted of 90 multiple-choice items, arranged in two sections of structure and written expression (40 items) and reading comprehension (50 items). The estimated reliability of the test (in this study) was α = .93, using Cronbach's Alpha.

4.2.2 The grammar test

A 20-item multiple-choice grammar test was developed by the researchers to be used as the pretest and posttest. The test was used for evaluating the participants' grammatical knowledge of a specific English structure, that is, conditional clauses. To attain the above-mentioned aim, first, a blueprint of a 30-item, multiple-choice test containing detailed item specifications was developed. It was, then, examined by two experts (i.e., two grammar instructors) to ensure its content validity. Upon receiving expert judgment, the test underwent the pilot-testing stage through being administered to 15 EFL freshmen students. It was, then, reduced to 20 items and was confirmed for its content validity. The test, further, went through the succeeding stages to ensure its reliability and concurrent validity. To this aim, a group of 60 undergraduate EFL students, who were all freshmen and at similar proficiency level to the main participants, took the test. The test enjoyed a satisfactory reliability estimate (α = .78), using Cronbach's Alpha.

Moreover, in order to ensure for the test’s concurrent validity, the (Pearson product-moment) correlation between the participants' scores on the grammar test and their performance on the structure section of TOEFL was computed. The results indicated a strong positive correlation (r = .89, p < .01), indicative of a satisfactory concurrent validity.

4.3.3 Measure of accuracy

In order to monitor learners' grammatical ability progression in conditional clauses in both SCMC and control groups in the current study, their L2 written outputs, produced while performing the three final tasks in the SCMC and classroom environments, were analyzed, measured, and compared using a well-known developmental measure of grammatical accuracy (i.e., error free T-unit ratio), deemed as a hypothetical anchor of language development by Wolfe-Quintero, Inagaki, and Kim (1998). According to Hunt (1970), T-unit is described as "a main clause plus all subordinate clauses and nonclausal structures attached to or embedded in it" (p. 4).

In language testing and SLA empirical studies, measures of grammatical accuracy are reported from two perspectives of global accuracy, that is, identifying any and all types of error, and specific types of error (Iwashita, Brown, McNamara, & O'Hagan, 2008). In the study, the second perspective, that is, specific types of errors, was employed. Accordingly, an error-free T-unit was defined as a unit containing the correct form of the special syntactic structures, that is, conditional clauses.

Additionally, to ascertain the significance of the findings, the study had to provide appropriate estimates of reliability for the chosen measure. Therefore, from the entire data set (i.e., The three final tasks) designated for tracing and appraising the development of learners' grammatical ability, one task was randomly chosen to be scored twice by a second expert scorer in order to check the interrater reliability. To this end, Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients (adjusted-for-two-raters) were calculated, the result of which is summarized in Table 1.

Table 1

Adjusted Interrater Reliability of Grammatical Accuracy Measure

Feature

Measure

(Adjusted) Reliability

Grammatical Accuracy

Error-free T-unit Ratio (EFT/T)

.894**

 **p < .01

As Table 1 shows, the obtained degree of consistency between the two raters was high in the study, standing for the adjusted value of .894 for error-free T-unit ratio.

4.3 Instructional Materials

4.3.1 The grammatical structure

For the study, an English grammatical structure, that is, conditional clauses, was employed as the target of instruction. To this aim, a frequently-used grammar coursebook at Iranian universities, that is, English Grammar Digest (Aronson, 1984), was employed as the basis for introducing and representing the conditional clauses in English. The reason behind choosing this grammatical structure derives from the results of corpus studies (e.g., Biber, Conrad, & Cortes, 2004) on lexical bundles in academic prose and conversation. According to Biber et al. (2004), conditional clauses together with that clauses, WH-clauses, and causative adverbial clauses are among the most frequently occurring structures in English lexical bundles.

4.3.2 The Software Application

An instant messaging web application called Skype was employed as the SCMC tool for the study. It is a freemium voice-over-IP service which was mainly developed by Zennstrom and Friis in assistance with three programmers named Heinla, Kasesalu, and Tallinn and was finally released in 2003 (Thomann, 2006). Skype provides users with an opportunity to communicate with their peers by voice via a microphone, by video through a webcam, and by instant messaging over the Internet. In this study, instant messaging was chosen as the preferred medium for communication among the peers themselves and between the learners and the instructor.

4.3.3 Tasks and Activities

Generally, all the utilized tasks and activities were categorized into two major groups: a) adjusted preparatory web activities and b) modified instructor-made tasks. The first group mostly involved the activities and exercises which are already available on the web. They were mainly employed as preparatory activities which aimed at both familiarizing all learners with the specified grammatical structure (i.e., conditional clauses) and initiating the SCMC group learners with the new environment of Skype and preparing them to work within it. To this purpose, therefore, a number of three preparatory activities were first selected by the instructor (one of the researchers) to be shared among the learners in the two groups (i.e., SCMC and control) and were then adjusted to meet the requirements of the two learning environments, that is, SCMC and traditional classroom. Examples of these activities included unscrambling words to make meaningful sentences using the appropriate forms of conditional clauses, playing a digital game with a focus on the specified structure, and describing images by making conditional sentences (Appendix A).

The second group, however, included instructor-made tasks which were subsumed under the broader category of structure-based production tasks. The latter involves the kind of grammar tasks through which learners carry out various production exercises and thereby internalize specific target structures (Nassaji & Fotos, 2011). Using the existing materials on the web and those within textbooks, the instructor developed three tasks to be employed in the final sessions of the treatment in the two groups. It is worth mentioning that the tasks were so adapted that they can be performed by the learners in the two learning environments (i.e., SCMC and classroom). In the following, two examples of these task types are explicated. In one task, the learners were provided with four pictures among which they were to choose one picture. They were, then, required to give an imaginary situation to the chosen picture and make as many conditional clauses as they could on the situation. In another task, they were provided with a number of single-paragraph short stories about several individuals, each explicating an event which had happened in their lives. The learners were, then, required to make a number of conditional clauses relevant to each story (Appendix B).

4.4 Procedure

Both qualitative and quantitative data collection procedures were employed. Owing to institutional constraints, random selection of the participants was not possible. Therefore, two intact grammar classes, each consisting of 30 male and female learners, were randomly assigned to experimental (SCMC) and control groups in Shahrekord University. Then, the TOEFL was administered to the participants of the two groups. The results of an independent sample t-test indicated no significant difference between the mean scores of the two groups in terms of English language proficiency (t(50.7) = .291, p = .77, p > .05). In the next session, the grammar test was administered as the pretest to the learners of the two groups.

After taking the pretest, the control group learners were exposed to a task-based noncollaborative approach to grammar instruction in a traditional teacher-fronted context. In the first session of the instruction, the instructor illustrated the conditional clauses to the learners clearly. For all subsequent sessions, they were presented with six relevant tasks and activities to be performed individually during a definite time span in the classroom. The first three activities were of the preparatory type and the last three ones were instructor-made tasks designated to trigger learners' written L2 output. The control group treatment continued for a total of seven sessions (each for 60 minutes), excluding TOEFL, pretest, and posttest administration sessions.

As for the experimental (SCMC) group, the learners experienced a task-based collaborative approach to grammar instruction in which they were provided with various affordances of an SCMC tool (i.e., Skype) in order to collaboratively perform the same tasks and activities which were given to the control group learners. At the beginning of the treatment, an introductory session was held to give the learners some basic information on how to use Skype and to create their own profiles in it. For all coming sessions, they were required to sign in Skype and be in contact with each other and with their instructor in groups of two or three. Prior to each session, the instructor set a specific day and time for two or three learners to sign in Skype. They, then, together made a group and held a text chat conferencing session in which the learners were required to work collaboratively and have interactive exchanges, through the available affordances of Skype, in order to carry out a specific task within the same time period given to the learners in the control group. Table 2 displays the different phases of data collection procedure in the SCMC group:

Table 2

Different Stages of Data Collection Procedure in the Experimental Group

Week

Sessions

Activities

Time

1

1

TOEFL   administration

90 Mins

1

2

Pretest   administration

15 Mins

2

3

Introductory   session

60 Mins

3

4

Preparatory   activities:

Task 1:   Unscrambling words to make meaningful sentences using the appropriate forms   of conditional clauses

60 Mins

4

5

Preparatory   activities:

Task 2:   describing images by making conditional sentences

60 Mins

5

6

Preparatory   activities:

Task 3:   Playing a ready-made digital game

60 Mins

6

7

Instructor-made   tasks:

Task 4:   Writing as many conditional clauses as possible for a number of   single-paragraph short stories

60 Mins

7

8

Instructor-made   tasks:

Task 5:   Listening to three short audio files and writing several sentences for each   situation containing relevant conditional clauses

60 Mins

8

9

Instructor-made   tasks:

Task 6:   choosing a picture from among several others, giving it an imaginary   situation and making as many conditional clauses as possible

 

9

10

Posttest   administration

15   Mins

Task performance in an SCMC environment required the learners to be in constant negotiated interaction and collaboration with each other in order to perform the specified tasks. Furthermore, they had the instructor as the coordinator and the one who was ready to guide learners and help them by scaffolding various opportunities for active negotiation and collaboration. This, in turn, provided the learners with plenty of opportunities to use the particular structure for meaningful purposes, to notice the forms they were using, to enjoy the instructor immediate feedback on their output, and, finally, to enhance their learning processes through active collaboration and coconstruction of L2 forms. After the treatment, the learners in both control and experimental groups were given the aforementioned grammar test as the posttest.

To measure and compare the probable development of L2 learners' grammatical ability, their L2 written outputs produced during the task performance phase in the two groups were analyzed. To this aim, first all output units (clauses & T-units) of each individual learner in both experimental and control groups were hand tagged on each of the three final tasks (i.e., Tasks 4, 5, & 6) and subsequently tallied. Clauses were determined according to Quirk, Greenbaum, Leech, and Svartvik’s (1985) definition and considering Neumann's (2014) two revisions: (a) all clauses consist of at least one finite or non-finite verb (i.e., verbless clauses were excluded) and (b) all verbs had to be accompanied by at least one other constituent; otherwise, they were considered part of another clause. T-unit, as mentioned earlier, was delineated as "a main clause plus all subordinate clauses and nonclausal structures attached to or embedded in it" (Hunt, 1970, p. 4). To measure the learners' probable trajectories of grammatical ability in the next step, each learner's error free T-units in terms of conditional clauses were hand tagged and tallied in order to calculate grammatical accuracy, that is, error free T-units per total number of T-units ratio. Finally, the collected data from the two groups underwent further statistical analysis to yield the intended results.

In the end, the participants of the SCMC group were interviewed with an open-ended question. The collected data from these interviews together with the data obtained from the same participants' written diaries during the treatment were examined qualitatively to reflect their perspectives on employing an approach to grammar instruction which involved task-based collaborative dialogues in an SCMC environment.

5. Results

The study pursued the three following main objectives: firstly, it examined the probable superiority of task-based computer-mediated collaborative interactions over a traditional noncollaborative approach in improving EFL learners' grammatical knowledge of conditional clauses, secondly, it traced, measured and compared the probable development of SCMC and control group learners in terms of grammatical ability of conditional clauses, and thirdly, it reflected EFL learners' attitudes towards employing task-based CMC-oriented collaborative interactions for improving their grammatical knowledge and ability.

5.1 Grammar Test Results

In order to explore the effect of task-based SCMC-oriented collaborative dialogues on EFL learners' grammatical knowledge of conditional clauses, both preliminary descriptive statistics and subsequent statistical analysis were carried out. Table 3 displays the descriptive statistic results of the grammar test scores in both pretest and posttest administrations for the control and the experimental groups within the study:

 

 

 

 

Table 3

Descriptive Statistics for Groups' Pretest and Posttest

Group

Test

N

Min

Max

Mean

SD

Skewness

Kurtosis

Control

 

Pretest

30

1

13

6.60

3.39

.22

-1.03

Posttest

30

4

14

8.30

2.70

.46

-.62

Experimental

 

Pretest

30

2

14

7.60

2.76

.39

.26

Posttest

30

12

20

16.03

2.15

.13

-.68

 

As demonstrated in Table 3, the skewness and kurtosis values were well within a satisfactory range of ±2, implying, in turn, the normal distribution of the data. Furthermore, the groups' mean scores ranged from 6.60 with a standard deviation of 3.39 for the control group at the pretest to 16.03 with a standard deviation of 2.15 for the experimental group at the posttest. A closer look at the results revealed fairly similar mean scores for the two groups on the pretest including (M = 6.60, SD = 3.39) and (M = 7.60, SD = 2.76) for the control and the experimental groups, respectively. Over time, however, the mean scores came to be different on the posttest, a fact which could be due to the influence of the intervention, so that the control group yielded a mean of 8.30 with the standard deviation of 2.70 while the posttest mean score for the experimental group was 16.03 with a standard deviation of 2.15.

A one-way between-groups analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was employed in order to evaluate the significance of the difference between the groups' posttest mean scores. The independent variable, being titled as Group in the analysis, was the type of pedagogical instruction (noncollaborative task performance & SCMC-based collaborative interactions). EFL learners’ posttest scores on the grammar test were labeled as the dependent variable and their pretest scores were included as the covariate in the analysis to control for preexisting grammatical knowledge differences between the groups. In order to make certain that there was no violation of underlying assumptions including normality, homogeneity of variance, linearity, and homogeneity of regression slopes, some preliminary checks were first conducted. Table 4 shows the ANCOVA results.

Table 4

 One-Way Between-Groups ANCOVA for Groups' Grammatical Development

Source

Type   III Sum of Squares

df

Mean   Square

F

Sig.

Partial   Eta Squared

Corrected   Model

1144.000a

2

572.000

324.958

.000

.919

Intercept

512.839

1

512.839

291.348

.000

.836

Pretest

246.934

1

246.934

140.285

.000

.711

Group

729.185

1

729.185

414.256

.000

.879

Error

100.333

57

1.760

 

 

 

Total

10126.000

60

 

 

 

 

Corrected   Total

1244.333

59

 

 

 

 

a. R Squared = .919 (Adjusted R Squared = .917)

As it is evident from Table 4, the results confirmed the existence of a significant difference among the two groups' post-intervention scores on the grammar test, F (1, 57) = 414.256, p = .000, p < 0.05. The partial eta squared value was found to be about 0.879, suggesting a large effect size for the posttest difference using Cohen's (1988) guidelines (.01 = small .06 = moderate .14 = large). Therefore, it can be safely claimed that there is a significant difference among the groups' posttest mean scores which was mainly due to the type of instruction employed for each group. That is to say, task-based collaborative dialogues in an SCMC environment positively influenced learners’ construction and development of L2 grammatical knowledge of conditional clauses.

5.2 Grammatical Ability Assessment Results

To measure and compare the grammatical ability of learners in SCMC and control groups, both descriptive and statistical analyses were conducted. Table 5 shows the descriptive results of assessing participants' L2 written outputs, employing the grammatical accuracy measure, that is, error free T-unit ratio:

Table 5

   Descriptive Statistics of Learners’ Grammatical Ability

 

 

Mean

SD

Total

Experimental

T-units

12.86

2.40

386

Error   free T-units

10.36

2.35

311

%   Error free T-units

79.80

8.96

 

Control

T-units

13.50

1.75

405

Error   free T-units

5.66

1.21

170

%   Error free T-units

41.56

7.14

 

 

According to Table 5, the means of error free T-units in experimental and control groups were 10.36 (SD = 2.35) and 5.66 (SD = 1.21), respectively, yielding the mean percentages of error free T-unit ratios which equaled to 79.80 (SD = 8.96) and 41.56 (SD = 7.14) for experimental and control groups, respectively. A closer look at the results revealed an apparent difference in the ratio means in favor of the experimental group, indicating a higher grammatical ability of its learners as compared with the control group ones. More inferential statistics, however, were required to confirm the significance of the mean difference.

To the above-mentioned aim, therefore, an independent sample t-test was conducted. The independent variable was the group type (i.e., experimental/control) which was titled as Group in the analysis, and the learners' grammatical ability index was considered as the dependent variable. Preliminary checks were first carried out to ensure that there was no violation of underlying assumptions including normality and homogeneity of variance. Table 6 illustrates the results.

Table 6

 Independent Samples t-test for the Grammatical Ability between

 the two Groups

 

t

df

Sig.a

Mean difference

Grammatical ability

18.27

58

.000

38.23

a two-tailed at .05

As Table 6 shows, the ratio means difference between the experimental and control groups for the grammatical ability index was statistically significant (t(58) = 18.27, *p < .05). The magnitude of the differences in the means was large (mean difference = 2.09, 95% CI: 34.04 to 42.42) with an eta squared value of 0.85. In sum, it can be inferred that the experimental (SCMC) group learners displayed higher trajectories of development in terms of grammatical accuracy which, in turn, implied their superior grammatical ability as compared with the control group learners.

5.3 Diaries and Interviews: A Qualitative Analysis

As mentioned earlier, EFL learners in the CMC-oriented (i.e., experimental) group were interviewed after the treatment with an open-ended question. They were asked to express their attitudes and explain their rationale about employing task-based collaborative interactions in an SCMC environment for the very purpose of grammar instruction. In addition, they were requested to keep diaries during the treatment in order to put forward their views on learning experience in CMC-oriented virtual environment, to evaluate the quality of the presented tasks in each session, and, finally, to pinpoint the perceived problems and the existing challenges of a task-based computer-mediated instruction in the current educational system. Interpretative analysis was carried out to reflect the important relevant themes from the learners' comments in diaries and interviews.

5.2.1 Attitudes and perceptions

Almost all of the learners in the experimental group favored a task-based approach to grammar instruction using collaborative dialogues in an SCMC environment. They admitted that the computer-mediated project was an innovative, interesting, and beneficial experience through which they could put their grammatical knowledge into use. They believed that integrating technology into the educational system would help the learners to be up-to-date and to enjoy the affordances of one of the world's recent technological advancements in education. Moreover, some others enjoyed the very fact that they could notice the errors and mistakes in their output through the instructor’s and the peers' feedbacks and comments. More interestingly, some of them stated that when they could work together and comment on their peers' writing tasks, they would become aware of their own mistakes. In other words, collaborative work with the peers and commenting on their output had provided the learners with opportunities to improve their own learning process. As far as the SCMC tool was concerned, some of the learners were pleased that they could see their sentences while chatting, a fact which had made them notice the words they were using. In the following, a few typical examples of learners’ comments on the CMC project are presented:

  • Mahtab, 22, female, the SCMC group:

                I believe that the project was really helpful and beneficial. I already had learned the conditional clauses through the traditional methods, but in this project, we had a chance to do various tasks with the help of our classmates and we could use the structures in several situations.

  • Ali, 21, male, the SCMC group:

                I think it was so interesting that I could comment on my classmates' sentences when we were doing the tasks together. It was very helpful and useful for me because my grammatical knowledge improved in this way.

  • Parvin, 22, female, the SCMC group:

I always had problem in learning grammar. In my opinion, we cannot learn grammar structures by only studying and memorizing them. We should practice a lot and use them in communications with other or when we are doing the tasks. It worked for me.

5.2.2 Challenges and problems

A number of learners also pointed out the problems they experienced during the CMC project. Their primary challenges were mainly related to technological issues such as difficulty in having access to a high-speed Internet connection and sometimes a general lack of sufficient rudimentary computer knowledge. The following examples demonstrate the learner’s opinions on the issue:

  •  Amir, 19, male, the SCMC group:

                I think it is a good way to learn the grammar but there were some problems about the low speed of Internet.

  •  Maryam, 20, female, asynchronous group:

                I think it is a new program that need time to be used by everybody and everywhere. The users should be up-to-date and have good computer knowledge. But its variety is more than the traditional system of teaching and learning.

6. Discussion

The study was firstly an attempt to examine the possible effect of two instructional interventions (i.e., task-based CMC-oriented collaborative interaction & task-based noncollaborative instruction in a traditional classroom) on enhancing EFL learners’ grammatical knowledge of conditional clauses. In addition, it sought to trace, measure, and compare the probable development of learners' grammatical ability in terms of conditional clauses through the mentioned approaches in two specified groups (i.e., SCMC and control). Finally, the participants of the SCMC-oriented group were interviewed after the treatment to give their perspectives on employing a task-based computer-mediated collaborative approach to grammar instruction.

As far as the first objective of the study was concerned, the results indicated that task-based collaborative interactions via an SCMC tool improved EFL learners' grammatical knowledge of conditional clauses. The visual display characteristic of SCMC drew learners' attention to linguistics forms and encouraged collaborative knowledge construction among them. The findings seem to be consistent with those from previous studies in which CMC-oriented collaboration drew L2 learners' conscious attention to language forms (e.g., O'Rourke, 2005; Zeng & Takatsuka, 2009).

Several factors contribute to the efficiency of SCMC-oriented collaborative work within the current study. First, SCMC-based courses, as practical instances of computer-mediated instruction, integrate the benefits of traditional classroom-based instruction with various affordances of virtual environments and provide EFL learners with unique situations to use the target language for meaningful purposes through collaborative efforts and exchanges. Traditional classroom-based courses have the advantage of an expert L2 user called instructor who can teach the target language effectively and help learners improve their L2 proficiency by providing them with suitable situations to practice the target language. Various affordances of virtual environments, on the other hand, enable L2 learners to be in contact with each other anytime and anywhere, and, as Salmon (2003) states, give them an opportunity to create their own learning communities through which they can learn together, take advantage of other learners' ideas, have access to different information resources, and enjoy the assistance of skillful mediators and mentors.

Second, as far as SCMC linguistic features are concerned, several researchers (e.g., Abrams, 2003; Yates, 1996) believe that SCMC is located in a continuum between the spontaneous oral language at one extreme and the formal written form at the other. That is to say, SCMC has a combination of features available in both settings, a unique characteristic which can turn SCMC into a considerably great platform for learning. Based on the above-mentioned argument, SCMC, as Salaberry (2000) states, can make a connection between focus on meaning and focus on form. In an oral discussion, learners are mainly concerned with getting their meaning across whereas during the interaction through the written form, they are inclined to zero in on language forms. Due to the very fact that SCMC shares the features of both written and oral contexts, it can create an optimal balance between form and meaning and, consequently, facilitate L2 learners' language development.

Finally, many SLA scholars (e.g., Anton, 1999; Belz, 2002; Cook, 2001) believe that learning an L2 is facilitated when L2 learners are provided with interactional opportunities in which the instructor guide and assist them in their communicative efforts and at the same time draw their attention to L2 forms. Distinctive characteristics of SCMC environment make it surpass many perceived limitations of teacher-fronted classrooms and set the scene for any FFI which is facilitated through instructor-scaffolded and also peer-scaffolded interactions and communication. According to Kern, Ware, and Warschauer (2004), the visual display of learners' utterances during the communication in CMC environments "allows greater opportunity to attend to and reflect on the form and content of the communication" (p. 244). It, therefore, enables the instructor or other peers to call the learners' attention to whatever structures, statements, or forms they intend.

The results of investigating the second concern of the study revealed that task-based collaborative interaction in an SCMC environment triggered EFL learners’ grammatical ability development more than a noncollaborative task-based approach in a traditional teacher-fronted classroom.

One plausible explanation for the above results bears on the temporal characteristic of SCMC mode. When interacting through SCMC tools, learners are usually limited to a short time-frame during which they have to respond to their communication partners, a fact that leads to "keeping learners within their grammatical comfort zone" (Stockwell, 2010, p. 99). In other words, as Stockwell mentions, learners prefer to draw on those grammatical forms and structures that they perceive to be accurate at the time and avoid using the structures they are not sure of. This very reason also prevents them from making more complex structures and make them to produce simpler and, therefore, shorter forms and structures which also give them a feeling of being more accurate.

Another explanation may relate to the presence of the instructor during the interactions and task performance processes within the study. In fact, the presence of the instructor may have had reduced the learners' risk-taking behavior which, in turn, have had prompted them to take care when producing their language outputs and, therefore, to avoid any probable embarrassment.

In addition, the fact that EFL learners' outputs in control group yielded a lower rate of grammatical accuracy as compared with those of SCMC (i.e., experimental) group learners may be due to several reasons, one of which seem to suffice for the purpose of the current argument. In the context of a classroom, learners have generally more opportunities, as compared with the context of an SCMC environment, to plan and edit their L2 texts before submitting them to their instructor. Consequently, their L2 outputs should contain greater accuracy than those produced in SCMC environments. This may hold true in case learners have achieved a level of language proficiency by which they can identify their errors when critically examining their written works. In many cases, however, they may lack the required knowledge and skills to find their errors and correct them, a fact which underlines the importance of various affordances within CMC environments which enable the instructor and more knowledgeable peers to give comments on learners' L2 written texts and, therefore, to help them notice their errors. Nonetheless, because of various constraints of teacher-fronted classrooms, as mentioned earlier, instructors cannot support their learners with instructional tips and corrections at appropriate moments which may render their feedback and comments inefficient for enhancing the accuracy of learners' L2 output as much as the instructor's or peers' immediate feedback can do in SCMC-oriented contexts.

Overall, as far as L2 learners' grammatical ability development is concerned, the results of the current study agree with the findings of Stockwell's (2010) study, on the one hand, and contrast with the results reported by Sotillo (2000) and Hwang (2008) on the other, in a way that language learning and interaction in an SCMC environment was influential in triggering EFL learners' grammatical accuracy, which, in turn, indicated their grammatical ability development in the current study. The finding also confirms the results of examining the first research question within the current study in which SCMC-based task performance and collaboration led to the development of EFL learners' grammatical knowledge compared with the traditional teacher-fronted context of the control group.

As far as the second research question of the study was concerned, the results of interviews from EFL learners in CMC-oriented group indicated that most of the learners had unanimously positive attitudes toward employing a task-based computer-mediated collaborative approach to grammar instruction.

Based on learners' comments, the following plausible explanations can be put forward for the appeal of the CMC-oriented instruction within the current study. First of all, CMC was reported to provide learners with opportunities to put their grammatical knowledge into use. Second, through the affordances of CMC environment, the learners had a sense of being members of a learning community within which they could associate with one another and help improving each member's L2 learning abilities through collaborative interaction. Third, compared with the traditional teacher-fronted contexts, CMC-oriented approach provided learners with more interactive opportunities, which, in turn, helped them to notice their error, to be forced to modify their output, and finally to strengthen their existing linguistic knowledge. Finally, the learners reported that they felt a sense of being up-to-date and kept the same pace with the rest of the world when such a technology-mediated approach was integrated into their regular academic course.

7. Conclusion

Mostly influenced by the premises of sociocultural theory, the study set out to examine the effect of task-based CMC-driven collaborative interaction on EFL learners' grammatical knowledge development. The results indicated that learners' collaborative efforts and exchanges in form of text chats during the task performance in an SCMC environment indeed enhanced learners' grammatical knowledge of a specified English structure, that is, conditional clauses. The second aim of the study was to trace, measure, and compare the learners’ trajectories of grammatical ability development in terms of conditional clauses in the SCMC (i.e., experimental) and control groups. The results suggested higher levels grammatical ability on the part of EFL leaners in SCMC group compared with their counterparts in the control group. Finally, the results of the qualitative analysis carried out on learners' interviews and diaries revealed that almost all of the EFL learners within the study favored a task-based computer-mediated collaborative approach to grammar instruction.

The findings have important pedagogical implications, especially for teachers and administrators in the EFL contexts. As a results of rapid development in the field of communication technology, CMC has now become a powerful tool which can help teachers to restructure the dynamics of the classroom, to provide EFL learners with ample opportunities to put their L2 knowledge into use, to create supportive and collaborative learning environments for learners, to encourage them to form electronic learning communities, and, finally, to enhance the process of learners' L2 learning.

Abrams, Z. I. (2003). The effect of synchronous and asynchronous CMC on oral performance in German. The Modern Language Journal, 87(2), 157-167.
Abrams, Z. I. (2008). Sociopragmatic features of learner-to-learner computer-mediated communication. CALICO Journal, 26(1), 1-27.
AbuSeileek, A. F., & Qatawneh, K. (2013). Effect of synchronous and asynchronous computer-mediated communication (CMC) oral conversations on English language learners' discourse functions. Computer & Education, 62, 181-190.
Aljaafreh, A., & Lantolf, J. P. (1994). Negative feedback as regulation and second language learning in the zone of proximal development. Modern Language Journal, 78(4), 465-483.
Anton, M. (1999). The discourse of a learner-centered classroom: Sociocultural perspective on teacher-learner interaction in the second-language classroom. The Modern Language Journal, 83(3), 303-318.
Appel, C., & Mullen, T. (2000). Pedagogical considerations for a web-based tandem language learning environment. Computers & Education 34(3-4), 291-308.
Aronson, T. (1984). English grammar digest. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Belz, J. A. (2002). Social dimensions of telecollaborative foreign language study. Language Learning & Technology, 6(1), 60-81.
Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Cortes, V. (2004). If you look at …: Lexical bundles in university teaching and textbooks. Applied Linguistics, 25(3), 371-405.
Blake, R. (2000). Computer mediated communication: A window on L2 Spanish interlanguage. Language Learning & Technology, 4(1), 120-136.
Chapelle, C. A. (2001). Computer applications in second language acquisition: Foundations for teaching, testing, and research. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Chenoweth, N. A., & Murday, K. (2003). Measuring student learning in an online French course. CALICO Journal, 20(2), 285-314.
Cheon, H. (2008).Sociocultural theory and computer-mediated communication-based language learning. Working Papers in TESOL & Applied Linguistics, 8(1), 1-3.
Cohen, J. W. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cook, V. (2001). Second language teaching and learning. New York: Arnold.
Ellis, R. (2001). Investigating form-focused instruction. Language Learning, 51(Suppl. 1), 1-46.
Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based language learning and teaching. Oxford University Press.
Foster, P., & Ohta, A. S. (2005). Negotiation for meaning and peer assistance in second language classrooms. Applied Linguistics, 26(3), 402-430.
Hsiao, J. W. D. L. (1996). CSCL theories. Retrieved from http://viking.coe.uh.edu/~shwang/master_portfolio/social%20constructivism/Hsiao.doc
Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in school children and adults. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 35(1, Serial No. 134).
Hwang, P. A. (2008). Linguistic characteristics in synchronous and asynchronous CMC. English Language & Literature Teaching, 14(2), 47-66.
Iwashita, N., Brown, A., McNamara, T., & O'Hagan, S. (2008). Assessed levels of second language speaking proficiency: How distinct? Applied Linguistics, 29(1), 24-49.
Jones, W. (2012). Assessing students’ grammatical ability. In C. Coombe, P. Davidson, B. O' Sullivan, & S. Stoynoff (Eds.), The Cambridge guide to second language assessment (pp. 247-256). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. 
Kelm, O. R. (1992). The use of synchronous computer networks in second language instruction: A preliminary report. Foreign Language Annals, 25(5), 441-454.
Kern, R. G. (1995). Restructuring classroom interaction with networked computers: Effects on quantity and characteristics of language production. The Modern Language Journal, 79(4), 457-476.
Kern, R., & Warschauer, M. (2000). Introduction: Theory and practice of networked language teaching. In M. Warschauer & R. Kern (Eds.), Network-based language teaching: Concepts and practice (pp. 1-19). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Kern, R., Ware, P., & Warschauer, M. (2004). Crossing frontiers: New directions in online pedagogy and research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 24, 243-260.
Kötter, M. (2002). Tandem learning on the Internet: Learner interactions in virtual online Environments (MOOs). Frankfurt, Germany: Peter Lang.
Kumar, V. S. (1996). Computer-supported collaborative learning: Issues for research. Eighth Annual Graduate Symposium on Computer Science, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatchewan, Canada.
Lantolf, J. P. (2000). Introducing sociocultural theory. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp. 1–26). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2011). Teaching grammar versus teaching grammaring. Retrieved from http://www.cornelsen.de/shop/capiadapter/download/get/
      file/%20pfu20110105.pdf?fileNr=1&sku=3-464-00072326-0&callerId=
      SBK3
Lee, L. (2002). Synchronous online exchanges: A study of modification devices on non-native discourse. System, 30 (3), 275-288.
Lee, L. (2004). Learners' perspectives on networked collaborative interaction with native speakers of Spanish in the US. Language Learning & Technology, 8(1), 83-100.
Lehtinen, E., Hakkarainen, K., Lipponen, L., Rahikainen, M., & Muukkonen, H. (1999). Computer supported collaborative learning: A review. Retrieved from http://comlab.hut.fi/opetus/205/etatehtava1.pdf
Loewen, S., & Erlam, R. (2006). Corrective feedback in the chatroom: An experimental study. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 19(1), 1-14.
McQuail, D. (2005). McQuail's mass communication theory (5th ed.). London: Sage.
Meskill, C., & Anthony, N. (2005). Foreign language learning with CMC: Forms of online instructional discourse in a hybrid Russian class. System, 33(1), 89-105.
Nagata, N. (1996). Computer vs. workbook instruction in second language acquisition. CALICO Journal, 14(1), 53-75.
Nagata, N. (1998a). Input vs. output practice in educational software for second language acquisition. Language Learning and Technology, 1(2), 23-40.
Nagata, N. (1998b). The relative effectiveness of production and comprehension practice in second language acquisition. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 11(2), 153-177.
Nassaji, H., & Fotos, S. (2011). Teaching grammar in second language classroom: Integrating form-focused instruction in communicative context. New York: Routledge.
Neumann, H. (2014). Teacher assessment of grammatical ability in second language academic writing: A case study. Journal of Second Language Writing, 24(1), 83-107.
Nunan, D. (2004). Designing tasks for the communicative classroom. Cambridge University Press.
Ohta, A. S. (2000). Rethinking interaction in SLA: Developmentally appropriate assistance in the zone of proximal development and the acquisition of L2 grammar. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp. 51-78). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
O'Rourke, B. (2005). Form-focused interaction in online tandem learning. CALICO Journal, 22(3), 433-466.
Ortega, L. (2009, September). Task and technology in language learning: Elective affinities and (dis)encounters. Plenary delivered at the Third International Task-based Language Teaching Conference, Lancaster, UK.
Pellettieri, J. (2000). Negotiation in cyberspace: The role of chatting in the development of grammatical competence. In M. Warschauer & R. Kern (Eds.), Networked-based language teaching: Concepts and practice (pp. 59-86). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. New York: Longman.
Richards, J. C., & Reppen, R. (2014). Towards a pedagogy of grammar instruction. RELC Journal, 45(1), 5-25.
Salaberry, M. R. (2000). L2 morphosyntactic development in text-based computer-mediated communication. Computer Assisted Language Learning, 13(1), 5-27.
Salmon, G. (2003). E-moderating: The key to teaching and learning online (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.
Smith, B. (2003). Computer-mediated negotiated interaction: An expanded model. Modern Language Journal, 87(1), 38-57.
Smith, B. (2005). The relationship between negotiated interaction, learner uptake, and lexical acquisition in task-based computer-mediated communication. TESOL Quarterly, 39(1), 33-58.
Sotillo, S. M. (2000). Discourse functions and syntactic complexity in synchronous and asynchronous communication. Language Learning and Technology, 4(1), 82-119.
Stahl, G., Koschmann, T., & Suthers, D. (2006). Computer-supported collaborative learning: A historical perspective. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 409-426). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Stockwell, G. (2010). Effects of multimodality in computer-mediated communication tasks. In M. Thomas & H. Reinders (Eds.), Task-based language learning and teaching with technology (pp. 83-104). New York: Continuum.
Storch, N. (2002). Patterns of interaction in ESL pair work. Language Learning, 52(1), 119-158.
Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 235-252). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp. 97–114). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Thomann, A. (2006). Skype: A Baltic success story. Retrieved from http://archive.is/FrClU
Thorne, S. L. (2003). Artifacts and culture-of-use in intercultural communication. Language Learning & Technology, 7(2), 38-67.
Van den Branden, K. (2006). Task-based language teaching in a nutshell. In K. Van den Branden (Ed.), Task-based language education: From theory to practice (pp. 1-16). Cambridge University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Warschauer, M. (1997). Computer-mediated collaborative learning: Theory and practice. The Modern Language Journal, 81(4), 470-481.
Wolfe-Quintero, K., Inagaki, S., & Kim, H. Y. (1998). Second language development in writing: Measures of fluency, accuracy, and complexity. Honolulu, HI: Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center, University of Hawaii at Manoa.
Yates, S. J. (1996). Oral and written linguistic aspects of computer conferencing: A corpus based study. In S. C. Herring (Ed.), Computer-mediated communication: Linguistic, social, and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 29-46). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Zeng, G., & Takatsuka, S. (2009). Text-based peer-peer collaborative dialogue in a computer-mediated learning environment in the EFL context. System, 37(3), 434-446.